Mind’s selective attention to previous experience

Authors

Antonis Theofilidis*, Filipos Kargopoulos
School of Psychology- Aristotle University of Thessaloniki

Article Information

*Corresponding author: Antonis Theofilidis, School of Psychology- Aristotle University of Thessaloniki.
Received: August 05, 2021
Accepted: August 27, 2021
Published: August 31, 2021
Citation:  Antonis Theofilidis, Filipos Kargopoulos. “Mind’s selective attention to previous experience”. Clinical Research and Clinical Case Reports, 2(1); DOI: http;//doi.org/04.2021/1.1029
Copyright: © 2021 Antonis Theofilidis. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

The basic arguments for a mental image model of thought are based on neuropsychological evidence. Farah (2000) found that the same brain regions are activated during both mental representation and actual perception, while Bishiah (1993) found that brain traumas that affected perception, also affected the ability to create mental images. Pylyshyn (2003) on the other hand, argues that all mental images are guided by ‘’cognitive penetrability’’, thus on their very basis, are manipulated by certain propositional elements. Given this difficulty, Kargopoulos (2009) hinted towards further research, featuring shapes and solid objects, for which subjects have no prior extensive knowledge. This would force subjects to use non-semantic strategies of representation, meaning mental imagery. Hinton’s (1979) cube problem conforms to these requirements. Hinton’s problem aligns with the idea that spatial tasks (especially tasks with cubes that change layout) are guided by propositional cues (our knowledge about squares) and supports Pylyshyn’s position. Using one of the simplest objects, a cube, Hinton showed that as soon as this shape changes its mental arrangement in space, even suspicious -as to the nature of the experiment- participants will make mistakes that are not present when they manipulate a mental image of the cube sitting on its typical array.
Aim: Our goal was to investigate the relationship between spatio-visual skill and the ability for mental partitioning in healthy subjects.
Methodology: We used 2 groups (344 participants) a control and an experimental one. In the control group, we presented a Moebius’ strip, in the experimental group, we presented the same Moebius’ strip and asked them to mentally represent it. All participants asked to  mentally partition the strip.
Results: Of the 344 participants, only 31 managed to give the correct number of vertices in space. Though people had a hard time manipulating the cube’s mental image, their success rates were much higher for the Hinton 1 task in which propositional representation was more accessible. Only 9 of the 344 participants could find the correct answer for the Moebius strip task in which mental manipulation of the strip image was impossible.
Conclusions: We come to the conclusion that the relationship between ‘’seeing’’ and ‘’knowing’’ is more complex, not just on the level of the mental image level but also on the level of perception. Our findings bring back to the scientific background the idea that the mind’s selective attention to previous experience and cognitive schemas will decidedly affect human thought.


Keywords: visual- spatial skill; mental image; cognitive schemas; mentall partition

Mental Partitioning

Although a lot of studies in cognitive psychology have researched mental object manipulation or transformation, there have been only a few attempting to locate the limits of mental partitioning in objects. Taking into account the limited research on mental partitioning, no study has examined the underlying mechanisms of mental partitioning, and the limits of it revolving simple, understandable objects for which subjects didn’t have prior knowledge (ex. Moebius’ strip), as well as its relationship to performance in spatial tasks. Research has mainly focused on spatio-visual and constructive-visual skills drawing from different theoretical backgrounds and utilizing a wide array of experiment designs, samples, analysis and data processing methods. In any case, there remains a basic challenge for cognitive psychology: Are there any mental partitioning tasks that may hint towards the nature of mental representations (solving the Kosslyn – Pylyshyn dispute) and is the theory of cognitive penetrability sufficient to establish a new propositional theory of perception?

Moebius’ Strip

Moebius’ strip is the first surface with one layer to be discovered and studied. It is a surface with only one side and one boundary. We can create a Moebius’ strip by cutting a strip from a piece of paper, rotating it by 180o, and then joining its ends to form a loop. Using a Moebius’ strip in mental representation studies allows us to examine people’s limits in space – perception tasks.

Method

The current study used an easily depictable shape for which subjects didn’t have much prior knowledge. Moebius’ strip conforms to the aforementioned characteristics, as it possesses a peculiar quality: partitioning the strip along its axon will not result in 2 separate pieces, but in a new Moebius’ strip, twice the length and half the width. Our first experiment tested whether language of thought, on the premise of it being the basis for all cognitive tasks, is exclusively digital or analogical utilizing mental images. We used 2 groups: a control and an experimental one. In the control group, we presented a Moebius’ strip and asked them to mentally partition it . In the experimental group, we presented them with a Moebius’ strip and asked them to mentally represent it, then we removed it from their field of perception, and when the subjects were sure that they could adequately represent it, we asked them to mentally partition it. We hypothesized that the same degree of success between the two groups would support Kosslyn’s views, whereas lower scores in the experimental group would support Pylyshyn’s. What happened was something we did not expect. After reviewing their scores, we found that there was a total failure in either of the two. The subjects’ failure to mentally partition Moebius’ strips led us to a new series of studies, focusing on the particularity of Moebius’ strip and the process of mental partitioning itself. We tried to investigate possible cognitive biases, a pattern of divergent judgements that is activated during mental partitioning and the effect of

«cognitive schemas» during this task. To summarize, we tested whether there are cases where problem solving is guided by established and unchanging «cognitive schemas», that affect not only mental images but also perception (Evans, 2010).

To investigate our hypothesis, we evaluated scores from two different tasks: Hinton’s cube (1979) and Moebius’ strip. Our basic goal was to answer:

  1. does established knowledge affect thought and perceptual ability negatively leading to cognitive biases,

  2. is Pylyshyn’s theory of cognitive penetrability applicable in the case of mental partitioning of an object (Moebius’ strip) and what are the differences between Pylyshyn’s and Kosslyn’s views on the nature of our mental representations,

  3. how do the scores in mental imaging tasks differ from scores in the mental partitioning ones. Our second goal was to investigate the relationship between spatio- visual skill and the ability for mental partitioning in healthy subjects, using the

Moebius’ strip, in order to examine the hypothesis that mental partitioning is a sub- skill of mental imaging presenting a high level of difficulty for subjects. Our third goal was to examine whether the influence of personal factors (such as: gender, age, level of education) affects performance in mental partitioning tasks and if scores of a certain group (psychology and architecture students) would be differentiated from the rest of the population.

Research Plan

We carried two studies: The first one attempted to answer our first goal. Initially, we divided our subjects into 2 sub-groups: a) a control group (N=186, 54.1%), in which subjects were presented with a Moebius’ strip and were asked to mentally partition the shape they had visual contact with, and b) an experimental group (N=158, 45.9%), in which subjects were presented with a Moebius’ strip while witnessing its creation process. After they confirmed that they could mentally represent it adequately, we removed the strip from their visual field and asked them to mentally partition its mental image. This plan attempted to investigate potential differences between perceptual and mental imaging process in mental manipulation tasks such as the one using the Moebius’ strip. To investigate whether, apart from mental images, people use propositional representations (Pylyshyn’s views), we administered 3 tasks:

  1. In the first task (Hinton 1), the correct answer was based on the subject’s prior knowledge on cubes (propositional representation).

  2. In the second task (Hinton 2), the correct answer was based on the mental manipulation of the cube (analogical representation).

  3. In the third task (Moebius’ strip mental partitioning), we hypothesized that the subjects’ prior knowledge (Whatever you cut gets separated into 2 pieces) would affect them and lead a significant percentage to answer incorrectly, supporting the view that this task involves mainly propositional and not analogical representations. The second study attempted to confirm our initial hypothesis that the mental partitioning of Moebius’ strip is guided by cognitive penetrability. It attempts to show that mental partitioning of Moebius’ strip, as a mental manipulation task, reaches the limits of human cognition and problem solving. Finally, the second study investigates the effects of three different sub-groups (1.architects, 2.psychologist, 3. Typical population) and of personal factors on spatial and partitioning problem solving.

Hypothesis

Study1: The basic hypothesis here is that there are spatial tasks, like mental partitioning of Moebius’ strip that don’t utilize mental images but are subjected to what Pylyshyn (2003) identified as cognitive penetrability. Taking into account that no participant had prior knowledge of Moebius’ strip, the scores in partitioning tasks would not be affected by the perceivable object but by their prior knowledge about how the world works. We predicted that the majority of the subjects would not put enough effort in the task using already established mental schemas and their properties. If the subjects could visualize the two simple shapes (Hilton’s cube and Moebius’ strip), then potential low scores in spatial tasks could hint towards increased difficulty in mental image manipulation. Specifically, regarding the mental partitioning of Moebius strip task answers such as «cutting the strip will give us 2 new strips»could support the idea that mental images depend on an established worldview, that is on abstract propositional knowledge and not on perceptual processes (Pylyshyn, 2003). Following the idea mentioned above, these two specific tasks (Hilton’s cube mental manipulation and Moebius’ strip’s mental partitioning) would be based on propositional representations, meaning that mental imaging processes are guided by propositional factors (Hypothesis 1). A second hypothesis states that the mental partitioning task would not be facilitated by the perceptually available Moebius’ strip (Hypothesis 2).

Study 2: Low scores in mental partitioning tasks are expected to confirm the existence of cognitive penetrability (Pylyshyn, 2003)(Hypothesis 1). High correlation is also expected between spatial perception and mental partitioning tasks (Anderson, 1981) (Hypothesis 3). We expect that gender will be a differentiating factor as Voyer, et. al. (1995) found that males score higher in certain spatial tasks, including mental partitioning (Hypothesis 4). Younger subjects are also expected to score higher than older ones (Silverman & Eals, 1992) (Hypothesis 5). Lastly we hypothesized that the group of architects would score higher than the other 2 groups (Voyer, et. al. 1995) (Hypothesis 6).

Study 1 – Participants

In the first part of the study a total of 344 subjects where tested. Of them 157 were male (45.6%) and187 were female (54.4%). Participants were categorized in 2 different age groups: above 25 years old (born in 1987 or earlier) (N=166, 48.3%) and below 25 years old (1988 and after) (N=178, 51.7%), in order to examine age factors in the tasks given.

Study 1 – Tasks

    1. Hinton 1 –Identifying the number of free edges of a mentally depicted Hilton’s cube

We asked the subjects to find the number of free edges in a mental depicted cube (30 cm each side) when that cube was standing on either of its edges

    1. Hilton 2 –Locating the free edges of a mentally depicted Hilton’s cube in space

The second task demanded the mental manipulation of the cube. We asked the subjects to pinpoint the cubes edges in space.

    1. Mental partitioning of Moebius’ strip task

We asked the subjects to imagine what the partitioning of a Moebius strip along its axon would give us. They were also given information about the strip’s creation process

Study 2 – Participants

A total of 154 subjects participated in this study. They were divided in 3 sub-groups : a ) a group of psychology students, b) a group of architecture students, c)and a typical population group. Specifically, 48 participants were architecture students (4th or 5th year) from the Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, 33 were psychology students (3rd year or above) from the Aristotle University of Thessaloniki and the rest came from typical population. Regarding gender, 77 were male and 77 female. Regarding age, 2 groups were formed : a) above 25 years old (N=77, M=53.8) and below 25 years old (N=77, M=21.2). 2 groups were also formed based on educational level : a)12 years or below (N=41), b) 13 years or above(N=113). Our sample did not include people with language or other problems (visual, auditory, psychopathology, etc.)

Study 2 – Tasks

  1. Spatial Ability Tasks

    1. Ropes: We presented our participants with a picture of 6 different ropes entangled together. We asked them to tell us if each rope would create a knot when pulled. Each success was awarded with 1 point

    2. Mental Paper Folding: We presented 20 instances where it was required from the participants to mentally fold a sheet of paper, pierce it and choose the correct solution from 5 alternatives as to how the holes would be arrayed if we unfolded that particular (Ekstrom, et. al., 1976). Each success was awarded with 1 point

    3. Hilton’s Cube – 2: The task is the same as the one described in Study 1. We awarded 14 points if all the edges were correctly arrayed in space (14 point = max score), 7 points if the participant arrayed at least 3 edges correctly and 0 points if the participant arrayed less than 3 correctly.

The total amount of points a participant could gather in the second study was 40.

  1. Mental Partitioning Tasks

The tasks administered were created specifically for the goals of the present study based on previous findings  (Kargopoulos, 2009). Cronbach’s  a was deemed sufficient (a=.81).

    1. Mental partitioning of the course of 2 trains along a Moebius’ strip: We presented our subjects with a Moebius’ strip (3cm width, 25cm length). We asked them to imagine the reverse lengthwise course of two train starting from six (6) different points of origin on the strip. The participants had to answer if the 2 trains would eventually meet on the strip. One point was awarded for each success

    2. Mental partitioning of the angle of an analogical clock’s indicators: We asked the participants to mentally partition the angle created by the

indicators on an analogical clock. The task had 20 items and one point was awarded for each success.

    1. Mental partitioning of a Moebius’ strip: We presented our participants with the process of creating a Moebius’ strip and asked them to describe the shape produced if we cut the strip lengthwise. One point was awarded for each success.

    2. Mental partitioning of a Moebius’ strip with alternatives: Each participant was presented with the strip and ask to mentally partition it lengthwise starting from a random point of origin. When they were finished they had to choose from 9 different alternative choices regarding the new shape produced. The answers were coded as «correct» or «incorrect» for the purpose of Binomial Test.

Statistical Analysis

We used the SPSS software package in order to analyze our data. To study the relationship between our variables we used the following techniques: one-way anova, binomial test, t-test to examine quantitive variables, chi squared independence test to examine categorical variables and adjusted standardized residuals to examine statistically significant chi-square results. For large samples, adjusted standardized residuals approach normal distribution. Positive adjusted standardized residuals >1.96 indicate statistically significant relationship between the variables in those cells.

From the 344 participants, 160 (46.5%) answered correctly in the Hilton 1 task , 31

(9%) answered correctly in the Hilton 2 task and 9 participant (2.6%) correctly partitioned Moebius’ strip (Table 1). Gender and age did not affect scores in the visual – spatial tasks (Hinton 1 and Hinton 2). Gender and age effects are present in the Moebius’ strip partitioning task. The correct answer was given by 6 males (1.7%) and 3 females (0.87%). Younger participants (<25) scored higher than the >25 group. From a total of 9 correct answers, 6 (1.7%) came from the younger group and 3 (0.87%) from the older. There were 7 correct answers in the control group (the group that were in visual contact with the Moebius’ strip) and 2 correct answers in the experimental one (Table 2).                                                                           

Tasks

Correct Answers                                                                                                                    

Percentage

False Answers

Percentage

Hinton 1

160

46.5%

184

53.5%

Hinton 2

31

9,00%

313

93,00%

Moebius' strip

9

2.6%

335

97.4%

Table 1. Frequency of answers in the tasks of the 1st study

Correct answers in Moebius' strip partitioning

 

Age

6

Male

 

3

Female

Gender

6

Below 25 years old

 

3

Above 25 years old

Experimental condition

7

Control Group

 

2

Experimental Group

Table 2. Frequency of correct answers (age*, gender*,experimental condition*, Moebius' strip mental partitioning*)

From these results we can infer that success rates in the mental partitioning tasks were low. A significant rate of success was present in the younger group. The group that had visual contact with the Moebius’ strip had a higher rate of success than the group that could only mentally manipulate without seeing it, however this result cannot support the idea that mental manipulation is facilitated by the perceptual condition Performance relationship to Hinton 1 and Hinton 2 tasks

To identify the statistical significance of the relationship between performance of the participants in Hinton 1 (Propositional representation) and Hinton 2 (Analogical representation) tasks, a chi-squared independence check was applied. There was a statistically significant correlation between performance in Hinton 1 and Hinton 2 tasks, chi-squared (N = 129, BE = 1) = 39.18, p <.001. Of the 160 participants who responded correctly to the Hinton 1 task , only 31 (24.03%) responded correctly to Hinton 2 (Analogical representation)(see Table 3).It seems that most people who responded correctly to the first task had mistakenly manipulated the mental image in the second one (Hinton 2). This confirms Hypothesis 1.

Hinton 1

False Answers

 

(4 vertices)

Correct Anwers

 

(6 vertices)

Total

 

Correct vertices array

0

31

31

Hinton 2                      False vertices array

184

129

313

Total

184

160

344

Table 3. Frequency of correct answers (Hinton 1*, Hinton 2*)

Performance relationship between mental partitioning of Moebius strip and Hinton 1 tasks

There was a statistically significant relationship in participants’ performance between mental partitioning and Hinton 1 tasks, chi-squared (Ν = 9, ΒΕ=1) = 10.62, p< .001. Those who responded correctly to mental partition tasks were also able to find the

correct number of vertices in Hinton 1 (Propositional representation) task (N=9) (see Table 4).

Hinton 1

False Answers

 

(4 vertices)

Correct Anwers

 

(6 vertices)

Total

 

Correct partitioning

0

9

9

Moebius' strip              False partitioning

partitioning

184

151

335

Total

184

160

344

Table 4. Frequency of correct answers (Hinton 1*, Moebius' strip mental partitioning*)

               Performance relationship between mental partitioning of Moebius strip and Hinton 2 tasks

There was a statistically significant relationship of participant performance between the Moebius strip and Hinton 2 tasks, chi-squared (Ν = 24, ΒΕ = 1) = 53.29, p < .001. Of the 31 subjects who responded correctly to the Hinton 2 (Analogical representation) task, 24 (77, 5%) gave the wrong answer to the mental partitioning task. This confirms Hypothesis 1 (see Table 5).

Hinton 2

False vertices array

Correct vertices

array

        Total

 

Moebius' strip

Correct partitioning

2

7

9

partitioning

 

False partitioning

 

311

 

24

 

335

Total

 

313

31

344

Table 5. Frequency of correct answers (Hinton 2*, Moebius' strip mental partitioning*)

              Relationship between the experimental conditions (mental partitioning of the strip) and performance in the Moebius strip task.

In order to examine the hypothesis that perceptual processing does not facilitate the mental processes in partitioning of the Moebius strip task (Hypothesis 2), we compared the performance of the two experimental groups. For the statistically significant relationship in the performance between the two groups to be identified, the chi-squared independence check was applied. There was a statisticaly significant correlation in performance between the experimental and control groups, chi-sqaured (N=156, BE=1) = 43.59, p<.001. Of the 186 subjects who had visual contact with the strip while trying to partition it mentally (control group), 179 (96.24%) gave the wrong answer and of the 158 subjects who tried partitioning its mental image (experimental group), 156 (98.74%) gave also the wrong answer (see Tabl

e 6). Binomial test for success in mental partitioning task showed that there’s a statistically significant difference in the percentage of those who failed in the control(p<.001) and the experimental groups (p<.001). The two groups showed no differences in their performance. This confirms Hypothesis 2.

Moebius' strip mental partitioning

 

False

 

partitioning

Correct

partitioning

     Total

 

Control Group

156

2

158

Experiemental condition      Experiemental Group

179

7

186

Total

335

9

344

Table 6. Frequency of correct answers in mental partitioning ( real Moebius' strip*, mental representation of Moebius' strip)

Relationship between age and performance in Hinton 1, Hinton 2 and mental partition of Moebius strip tasks.

According to the results from the first two tasks (Hinton1 and 2) there were no significant differences in performance amongst subjects from the two age groups. Of the 9 subjects who responded correctly to the Moebius strip task, the younger (under 25) showed better performance (6 subjects, 66.7%).The Binomial test for success or failure in each age group in Moebius strip task showed a statistically significant difference for younger subjects (p < .001).

Relationship between gender and performance in Hinton 1, Hinton 2 and Moebius strip tasks.

According to the results from the first two tasks (Hinton 1 and 2) there were no significant differences in performance between men and women. Of the 9 subjects, however, who responded correctly in the mental partitioning task, men, showed a better performance (6 subjects, 66.7%). The binomial test for success or failure between men and women in the Moebius strip task showed that there was a statistically significant difference over men (p < .001).

Summary of the results from the first study

According to the results from the first study, only 46,5% of the participants responded correctly in the Hinton 1 task . Assuming that when they tried to solve Hinton 1 task, people used a virtual mental representation, then we could say that it didn’t help them to answer correctly. We claim that the unusual position of the cube and its vertices makes its virtual representation a quite difficult task, we also claim that those who gave the correct answer either used the propositional representation: «The cube has eight vertices, if I remove two then six remain» or they were really good at manipulating the cube’s mental image. If the latter were true, then those participants should have showed a higher rate of success in the task that followed, which clearly required the mental manipulation of the inverted cube’s image (Hinton 2). It seems that there are spatial tasks that in order to be solved need not only the manipulation of a virtual representation but most importantly the use of some prior knowledge in the form of propositional representation. Subjects in their vast majority found it more difficult to manipulate the cube’s mental image in the second task. Of the 344 participants, only 31 managed to give the correct number of vertices in space. Though people had a hard time manipulating the cube’s mental image, their success rates were much higher for the Hinton 1 task in which propositional representation was more accessible. Only 9 of the 344 participants could find the correct answer for the Moebius strip task in which mental manipulation of the strip image was impossible. This task seems to have been catalyzed by the prior knowledge: «Whatever you cut gets separated into two pieces» (Cognitive penetrability) which led 97,3% of the participants to the wrong answer. With regard to the variables we examined in mental partitioning it seems that age and gender tend to influence performance with young (under 25) men being better, though the small number of participants who responded correctly prevents us from further supporting this case. One particular interest was the fact that the condition of having visual contact with the strip while trying to mentally partition it did not increase the percentage of correct responses significantly. This means that Pylyshyn’s cognitive penetrability should be also applied to perceptual phenomena (Kargopoulos, 2009). The experimental groups comparison shows that participants who were in visual contact with the Moebius strip showed a better performance, though these results cannot support that mental partition is facilitated by being in perceptual contact with the object.

Study 2

The relationship in overall performance between visual competence and mental partitioning tasks.

In order to investigate the correlation in overall performance between visual competence and mental partitioning tasks we applied Pearson correlation coefficient. The overall performance in visual competence tasks showed a statistically significant correlation with performance in mental partitioning tasks r = .75, p <.001. There was a positive correlation to performance in visual competence and mental partitioning tasks. This confirms Hypothesis 3.

The relationship between profession and overall performance in visual competence tasks.

Anova analysis to compare the mean scores in 3 visual competence tasks between the three different profession groups showed statistically significant results. Specifically, the effect of the profession factor was statistically significant [F(2.151) = 12.64, p = . 001]. After making adjustments according to Bonferroni, regarding the number of comparisons we found statistically significant differences between the mean scores of the professions, paired t = (104) = 1.37, two-way p< .001. The mean scores of the architecture group (Μ = 24.02, SD = 7.2) were significantly higher than those of the typical population (Μ = 18.88, SD = 7.03) and psychology groups alike (Μ = 16.97, SD = 5.57). The architecture students had higher scores than the psychology students and the people working typical jobs. This partially confirms Hypothesis 6.

Relationship between profession and overall performance in mental partitioning tasks.

Anova analysis to compare the mean scores in mental partitioning tasks between the three different profession groups showed statistically significant results. Specifically, the effect of the profession factor was statistically significant [F(2.151) = 3.04, p < . 005]. After making adjustments according to Bonferroni, about the number of the number of comparisons, we found statistically significant differences between the mean scores of the professions t(104) = 1.54, δίπλευρη p < .005). The mean scores of the architecture group (ΜΟ = 15.19, ΤΑ = 8.27) were significantly higher than those of the typical population (ΜΟ=12.37, ΤΑ=6.94 and psychology group (ΜΟ=11.58, ΤΑ=6.53). This confirms Hypothesis 7.

Relationship between age and overall performance in spatial reasoning tasks

Anova analysis to compare the mean scores in spatial reasoning tasks between the 2 age groups (above 25 and below 25) showed statistically significant results. Specifically, the effect of age factor was statistically significant (F151= 14.34, p< . 000). The mean scores for «below 25» age group (ΜΟ = 22.05, ΤΑ = 7.93) were statistically higher that those of the «above 25» group (ΜΟ = 17.76, ΤΑ = 5.73). Participants that were below 25 years old scored higher in spatial reasoning tasks.

Relationship between age and overall performance in mental partitioning tasks

Anova analysis to compare the mean scores in mental partitioning tasks between the 2 age groups (above 25 and below 25) showed statistically significant results.

Specifically, the effect of age factor was statistically significant (F152 = 6.96, p

<.000). The mean scores for «below 25 age» group (ΜΟ = 14.51, ΤΑ = 8.48) were statistically higher than that of the «above 25» group (ΜΟ = 11.41, ΤΑ = 5.49). Participants that were below 25 years old scored higher in mental partitioning tasks. This confirms Hypothesis 6.

Task : «Alternative solutions to mental partitioning of Moebius’ strip»

For the purposes of this study, the answers provided in the task were participants had to choose from 9 alternative solutions to the mental partitioning of Moebius strip, were of particular interest.

 

 

Alternative Answers

Frequency of answer                           

 

False percentage

2 large collars, one inside the other

8

        

5.2%

1 bigger Moebius' strip, half in width

8

 

5.2%

2 new Moebius' strips, one inside the other, half in width

5

 

3.2%

1 big simple collar

8

 

5.2%

2 separate Moebius' strips, same as the original, half in width

104

 

67.5%

1 Moebius' strip with 3 loops

1

 

0.6%

2 new seperate, non-connected, simple collars

2

 

1.3%

A cohesive piece, which I can't identify

0

 

0,00%

2 seperate pieces, which I can't identify

18

 

11.08%

Table 7. Frequency of answers in alternative answers task

Out of the 154 participants, 137 (89%) chose a wrong answer revolving around the separation of the strip into 2 distinct pieces (answers: 1, 3, 5, 7, 9). 17 participants (11%) chose answers that had only 1 piece as the product of mental partitioning (answers: 2, 4, 6, 8), and of the 17, only 8 (5,2% from a total of Ν=154) gave the correct answer (second option). Biniomial tests for the success or failure in the task of ’Alternative solutions to Moebius’ strip mental partitioning’ showed statistically significant differences over the participant’s failure rate(p < .001). The percentage of participants who were close to the correct answer was statistically smaller than the percentage of those who provided a wrong answer. This result confirms our hypothesis about the existence of strong cognitive biases and of cognitive penetrability in the specific task, affirming our initial hypothesis (Hypothesis 1). At this point we must stress out that the most chosen option among the alternatives provided was the propositional representation: «whatever you cut gets separated into2 new pieces». The alternative 5 option, according to which cutting the strip lengthwise will give 2 new strips of equal length but half the width of the original, gathered the highest percentage of answers (67.5%). The alternative answer 8 concerning one whole piece that participants couldn’t identify gathered 0%. This result supports the idea that participants used the propositional knowledge and didn’t put any effort in mentally manipulating the picture. We can also observe that among the 154 participants in the 2nd study, 137 (89%) chose between answers concerning 2 pieces and 17 participants (11%) chose between alternatives regarding 1 piece as a product of mental partitioning. This leads us to believe that prior established knowledge and not mental manipulation of images guided the majority of our sample in false answers regarding the mental partitioning task, making obvious the existence of cognitive biases guiding thought during this task’s problem solving process. This also confirms our 1st Hypothesis.

Summary of results from the second study

From the results of the 2ndstudy, we conclude that there is a significant relationship between performance in spatial and mental partitioning tasks. Architecture students and participants below 25 years of age scored higher in both tasks. It seems that age and profession affects spatial reasoning competence. Τhe task «Alternative solutions to Moebius’ strip mental partitioning» hints towards the use of propositional thought as well as the guidance of this task’s representation by prior knowledge about the world. Specifically, whereas in the first task, success rate was 2.6%, when alternative answers were provided it doubled (5,2 %).However, the success rate was still considerably low. A wide percentage of the answers provided (89%), revolved around the result of 2 separate pieces, leading us to believe that previous knowledge as to the nature of partitioning affected the participants’ answers. These results provide evidence that the ability to mentally partition an object is cognitively penetrable, but also hints towards a strong cognitive bias that affected a high percentage of our sample. The most common option chosen amongst alternatives hints towards our initial hypothesis: our inner propositional representation that cutting an object leads to 2 new seperate pieces will guide us towards the option : «2 new strips our produced» which gathered 67.5% of the total answers. These results provide evidence regarding that most of the participants used their propositional knowledge while putting little to no effort in mentally manipulating the strip.

Conclusions

The low rates of correct answers in the 2 studies (a total of 498 subjects, with only 2.6% of them finding the correct answer), for both participants who could see the strip as well as those who tried to mentally manipulate it, leads us to the conclusion that cognitive penetrability, as defined by Pylyshyn (2003), guides mental transformation. We can also expand the idea of cognitive penetrability to perception. The large failure rate in the mental partitioning tasks supports the idea that some mental transformation problems are not facilitated by prior knowledge. The participants didn’t seem to manipulate the strip’s mental image, and even if we accept that they manipulated it, the majority failed at the task. The basic principle of propositional representation is that words and images are represented in an abstract way that suggests the meaning and the use of knowledge (Pylyshyn, 2003). People encode and use all information (verbal and non verbal) in the form of propositional representations. When we want to use this information, we recall the corresponding propositional representation and use it for the appropriate tasks. The information: «Whatever you cut gets separated into 2 pieces» is a propositional unit that consists of two modules: 1st module = «Whatever you cut» and 2nd module = «two pieces» are connected by the relationship of «gets separated into». This rule seems to have affected the participants and validated Pylyshyn’s views that mental images are guided by propositional cognitive cues (cognitive penetrability), at least regarding the task of the mental partitioning of Moebius strip. The participants had also difficulties in mentally manipulating the perceivable strip. They mustered available cognitive schemas (a simple collar) and tried to create semantic relationships between the old and new knowledge in order to understand and find the solution to the task at hand. Whatever the participants tried to manipulate, concerning the Moebius strip task was affected by prior knowledge (the schema: «Whatever you cut , gets separated into 2 pieces»), which confirms Neisser’s theory (1997). Shepard & Metzler’s (1971) views, that people execute various mental tree – dimensional transformations cannot be supported by this study’s findings. The low success rates in the mental partitioning tasks lead us to the conclusion that the law of the least possible mental effort is true. People will use a spontaneous / fast way of thinking (Kahneman, 2011). The participants used a fast and spontaneous system of thinking, which works- based on associations, with little to no effort, without the use of self  regulation, based on cognitive biases, with limited use of typical logic, and for that reason people came to false conclusions. According to our results, the independent variables «age», «profession» and «education level» have an effect on spatial perception and mental partitioning tasks. The architecture students group and the «below 25» group scored the highest, and the psychology student group scored the lowest. The factor «education level» affects only the spatial perception tasks and not the mental partitioning ones, with higher education level participants scoring higher. Scores in spatial perception tasks are a predictor for performance in mental partitioning tasks and vice versa. The participants displayed a wide variety of cognitive behaviors during the tasks administered, that hinted towards limitations of human thought. Their answers were a product of prior knowledge (biases), which prevented them from using possibly more effective solution methods. Scores in the main mental partitioning task showed that spatial reasoning is affected by what people know, and not by what they see or manipulate. These results lead us to support the idea that the process of mental partitioning a novel, real object rests on propositional representations and not on visual images. The participants’ thought was anchored in the idea that cutting an object leads to 2 seperate pieces and prevented them from approaching the problem from a different perspective. In this specific partitioning sub-task, subjects sought a fast approach, that would connect their goal with available to them information. They used prior knowledge, on the basis that this would facilitate problem solving. People manipulating a Moebius’ strip seemed to see what they know, and not what ‘’there is’’. These findings support Pylyshyn’s ideas, who supported the presence of specific conceptual – shaping processing. Ιn particular the position that virtual processes are guided by cognitive propositional elements (Pylyshyn, 2003). It seems that a series of «up down» information processes affect our perception, whereas our experiences create our internal theories and the way we perceive the world, at least in the case of visual – constructive transformation tasks (ex. Moebius’ strip’s mental partitioning).The study of cognitive penetrability and the mental partition of mental and real object such as Moebius’ strip constitute fertile ground for further inquiries in Cognitive science. Our findings bring back to the scientific background the idea that the mind’s selective attention to previous experience and cognitive schemas will decidedly affect human thought.

References

  1.  
  2. Anderson, J. R. (Ed.). (1981). Cognitive skills and their acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  3. Bisiach, E . (1993). Perception and action in space representation : Evidence from unilateral neglect . In Current Adoonces in Psychological Science : An Internolional Perspective . Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hove and London, press .
  4. Ekstrom, R. B., French, J. W., & Harman, H. H. (1976). Manual for kit of factor- referenced cognitive tests. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.
  5.  
  6. Evans, J. D., (2010). Re-Entrant Corner Singularity of the PTT Fluid. In: Simos, T. E., Psihoyios, G. and Tsitouras, C., eds. Numerical Analysis and Applied Mathematics, Vols I-III. Vol. 1281. American Institute of Physics, pp. 1676-1679.
  7. Farah, M. J. (2000). The neural bases of mental imagery. InM. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.),
  8. The cognitive neurosciences (2nd ed., pp. 965–974). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  9. Hinton, G..E.(1979). Some Demonstrations of the Effects of Structural Descriptions in Mental Imagery. Cognitive Science.3, p.231-250.
  10. Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. Farrar, Straus and Giroux, ISBN 978- 0374275631.
  11. Καργόπουλος, Φ. (2009). Η συνείδηση μεταξύ γνωσιοεπιστήμης και νευροεπιστήμης. Noesis 5, 1-8.
  12. Moser, M. B.,& Moser, E. I. (2015).2014 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine. Journal of Investigative Medicine, 63(1).
  13. Pylyshyn, Z.W. (2003). Seeing and visualizing: It’s not what you think. Cambridge, M.A.: MIT Press.
  14. Shepard, R.N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three – dimensional objects. Science, 171 (3972), 701 – 703.
  15. Silverman I. & Eals, M. (1992). Sex differences in spatial abilities: evolutionary theory and data. In : Barkow JH, Cosmides L,Tooby J (eds) The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology the generation of culture. Oxford University Press, New York, pp.533 – 549.
  16. Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M.P.(1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities:A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin 117, p. 250-270.
  17.